用户名: 密码: 验证码:
3~4岁儿童攻击行为的多方法测评及其与“心理理论”的关系
详细信息    本馆镜像全文|  推荐本文 |  |   获取CNKI官网全文
摘要
攻击行为是儿童中普遍存在的一种社会行为,是儿童个性社会性发展的重要内容。攻击行为影响儿童人格和品德的形成,是个体社会化成败的重要指标。长期以来,儿童攻击行为一直是心理学特别是发展心理学的重要课题和研究热点。本论文由两项研究组成,研究一是《3~4岁儿童攻击行为的多方法测评研究》,研究二是《3~4岁儿童攻击行为与“心理理论”的关系》。
     儿童攻击行为测评的科学性、有效性和准确性直接决定着该领域研究的水平和质量,对测评方法的研究和评价从而成为该领域研究的基础和前提条件。儿童攻击行为的测评方法主要有问卷法、量表法、观察法、社会测量法、访谈法等。国内外已有的研究多采用单一方法测评儿童的攻击行为,常常导致研究方法的效度不高。研究一以整群抽样法选取177名3-4岁幼儿园小班儿童为被试,同时运用自然观察法、母亲评定法、教师评定法和同伴提名法,多方法多角度地评定儿童的攻击行为,采用潜变量测量模型检验儿童攻击行为的多方法测评与实测数据拟合情况,得出如下主要结论:
     (1)自然观察、教师评定、母亲测量和同伴提名具有较高的信度;
     (2)四种测评方法均具有较高的效标关联效度,同伴提名和教师评定的效标关联效度高于自然观察和母亲测量;
     (3)四种测评方法的测评模型与实测数据拟合良好,表明多方法测评是更为准确有效的攻击行为测评工具。
     儿童攻击行为的认知机制是该领域研究中的深层问题。道奇的社会信息加工模型是解释儿童攻击行为认知机制的著名理论,但某些攻击行为如间接攻击的实施客观上要求儿童具备相对较高的认知和操纵他人心理的能力,这是道奇的“认知缺陷假说”所无法解释的。儿童攻击行为的认知机制可能与攻击行为的类型相联系,属于身体、动作攻击型与以实施间接攻击为主的儿童的认知能力可能存在差异。因此,需要结合攻击行为的类型对攻击性儿童的认知特点进行研究。我们把这一观点称为攻击行为认知机制的“类型特殊性”假说。
     “心理理论”是指个体对他人和自己心理状态及其与他人行为的关系的一种常识性认知。研究二参照道奇的标准从研究一的被试中选取93名幼儿园小班儿童为被试,以自然观察法为儿童攻击行为的测评指标,采用“意外转移”和“欺
    
     骗外表”两种测验任务,从“心理理论”的角度探讨儿童攻击行为的认知机制。
     研究得出如下主要结论:
     (1)攻击儿童与无攻击行为儿童的心理理论能力不存在显著差异:
     *)间接攻击儿童的心理理论能力显著高于身体攻击和无攻击行为儿童,其
     他儿童之间不存在显著差异;
     *)各组儿童对自己心理的认知能力不存在显著差异;
     本论文的创新之处在于运用潜变量测量模型进行多方法多角度的测量分析,
     评定儿童攻击行为测评方法的信度和效度,建立评估儿童攻击行为的最忧方法和
     科学指标:通过现场实验证明,攻击儿童与无攻击行为儿童的心理理论能力不存
     在显著差异,至少是某些攻击几童(如间接攻击儿童)并不存在认知缺陷。
Aggression is a type of social behavior of relatively high prevalence among children. It is an aspect of children's personality and social development. Aggression is closely associated with the formation of children's personality and morality, and normally considered as an important index of individuals' socialization. The development of children's aggressive behavior is one of the most important research areas in developmental psychology. This dissertation is consist of two studies. Study one focused on the multi-method assessment of children's aggressive behavior between 3 to 4 years old. and study two explored the relationship of children's aggressive behavior to their theory-of-mind.
    Effective and accurate assessment of children's aggressive behavior determines directly level and quality of the research in the area. Therefore, the study and evaluation of methods for assessing children's aggressive behavior constitutes one of the most fundamental tasks for the research in this area. The main methods of measuring children's aggressive behavior include questionnaire, scale, observation, nomination, interview and so on. Most previous studies used solar method to measure children's aggressive behaviors, and often led to low validity of the result, and at times lead to difficulties in testifying the results from one another. In the first study, a panel of measures including natural observation, mother assessment, teacher assessment and peer nomination were employed in a sample of 177 3-4 years old children from three urban kindergartens. The validity and reliabilities of each method was evaluated for each measure. The main findings of study one were as follows:
    1. Natural observation, mother assessment, teacher assess and peer nomination each had comparatively high reliability. 2. Each of the four measures also had comparatively high criterion validity. 3. The theoretical multiple-method model using four measures was very well fitted to data, which proved to be a more accurate and effective measuring tool of aggression.
    The cognitive mechanism of children's aggressive behavior is a deep-seated issue for research. Dodge proposed the social information processing model to interpret the cognitive process under children's aggressive behavior. However, the
    
    
    
    conducting of certain type of aggressive behavior such as indirect aggression does require comparatively high cognitive ability to understand and manipulate others' mind on the part of the aggressor, which can't be explained by Dodge's cognitive deficiency hypothesis. We believe that the cognitive process under children's aggressive behavior may be relates to the types of aggressive behavior. The cognitive ability of those children categorized as indirect-aggression group may be different from that of the physical-aggressive children. So the cognitive feature of aggressive children must be examined according to the specific types of aggressive behavior. We would rather call this viewpoint type-specific hypothesis of cognitive mechanism of aggressive behavior.
    Theory-of-mind refers a naive cognition that individuals understand others' mental state and that of their own as well as the relationship of others' behavior to their mind. In the second study, the 'unexpected transfer' and 'deceptive appearance' test tasks were employed in a sample of 93 children selected from the first study, in an effort to explore the cognitive mechanism of children's aggressive behavior in terms of the ability of theory-of-mind. The results showed that, overall, no significant difference in the ability of theory-of-mind existed between aggressive and non-aggressive children. Indirect-aggressive children had a higher level of ability of theory-of-mind than those physical-aggressive children and non-aggressive children, but the latter two groups did not differ significantly. All the three groups of children had similar abilities of understanding their own mind.
    This study developed a multi-method model for measuring children's aggressive behavior and evaluated its reliabilities a
引文
1. Achenbach, T. M., Edelbrock, C. Manual for the child behavior checklist and revised child behavior profile. Burlington: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry. 1983
    2. Astington J., Harris, P., Olson, D. Development theories of mind. New York: Wiley. 1988
    3. Astington J.W., The Child's Discovery of the Mind, London: Fontana, 1994
    4. Baillargeon, R. Boulerice, B. Tremblay, R. et al. Child Psychological Psychiaology. 2001,42(4) : 463-473
    5. Bandura, A. Social learning theory. 2nd ed. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. 1977
    6. Bandura, A. The Mechanism of Aggression, in GG Russell. & Edward I. Donnerstem eds, Aggression, Academic Press, 1983:1-40
    7. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A.M. & Frith, U. Does the autistic child have a theory of mind? Cognition, 1985,21:37-46
    8. Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cohen, D. J., Understanding other minds: perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience,-2nd ed.-Oxford; New York: Oxford University Press, 2000
    9. Bjorkqvist, KL, Lagerspetz, KMJ, & Kaukiainen, A.: Do girls manipulate and boys fight? Developmental trends in regard to direct and indirect aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 1992; 18: 117-127
    10. Brain, P F. Hormonal aspects of aggression and violence. In A.J. Reis, Jr.&J.A.Roth(Eds.) Understanding and control of biobehavioral influences on violence (Vol. 2) . Washington, DC: National Academy Press. 1994,177-244
    11. Buss A. H. & Perry, M.. The aggression questionnaire. J Pers Soc Psychol, 1992, 63: 452-459
    12. Costabile A, Palermiti AL, Tenuta F. Una realita calabrese. In: Fonzi A. Ⅱ bullismo in Italia. Firenze:Giunti. 1997
    13. Craig W, Pepler D, Atlas R et al. observations of bullying in the playground and in the classroom. School Psychology. 2000, 21(1) :22-36
    14. Crick N.R., Dodge K A. A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children's social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 1994, 115: 74-101
    15. Dodge and Coie, Social-information-processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children's peer groups. Journal of personality and social psychology, 1987,53:1146-1158
    
    
    16. Dodge K. A. and Pettit G. S. and MaClaskey C L, Brown M M. Social competence in children. Monographs of the society for research in child development, 1986, 51(2) : 1-85
    17. Dodge KA, Coie JD, Pettitt GS, Price JM. Peer status and aggression in boy's groups: Developmental and contextual analyses. Child Development, 1990,61, 1289-1309
    18. Dodge, K. A. and Frame, C. L. Social Cognitive Biases and Deficits in Aggressive Boys. Child Development, 1982, 53: 620-635
    19. Dodge, K. A. Social Cognition and Children's Aggressive Behavior. Child Development, 1980, 51: 161-170
    20. Dollard, J. Frustration and Aggression, New Haven: Yale University Press. 1939
    21. Flavell, J. H., Development of children's knowledge about the mental world, international journal of behavioral development, 2000. 24(1) : 15-23
    22. Flavell, J.H., Miller, P.H. Social cognition. In D. Kuhn & R.S. Siegler (Eds.) , W. Damon (Series ED.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol.2. Cognition, perception, and language (5th ed.,). New York: Wiley. 1998, 851-859
    23. Gopnik, A., & Wellman, H.M.. The 'theory' theory. In L.A. Hirtschfeld & S.A. Gelman(Eds.),Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture. Cambrige, UK; Cambrige University Press. 1994,257-293
    24. Grotpeter J, Crick N. Relational Aggression, Over Aggression, and Friendship. Child Development. 1996,67:2328-2338
    25. Harris, G. From simulation to folk psychology; The case for development. Mind and Language, 1992,7:120-144
    26. Hartup, W. W. Aggression in childhood: Developmental perspectives. American Psychologyist, 1974,29:336-41.
    27. Lagerspetz, KMJ, Bjrkqvist, K, & Peltonen, T: Is indirect aggression typical of females? Gender differences in aggressiveness in 11-to 12-year-old children. Aggressive Behavior, 1988; 14: 403-414
    28. Leekam. S., Children's understanding of mind, Bennett. M., (Eds)The Child as Psychologist: An introduction to the development of social cognition,Harvester, 1993
    29. Lefkowitz, M. M., Eron, L. D., Walder, L. O., & Huesmann, L. R. Growing up to be violent: a longitudinal study of the development of aggression. New York: Pergamon. 1977
    
    
    30. Leslie, A.M. Pretending and believing: Issues in the theory of ToMM. Cognition, 1994,50:211-238
    31. Leslie, A.M. TOMM, TOBY, and agency; core architecture and domain specificity. In L.A. Hirschfeld & S.A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. 1994, 119-148
    32. Loeber R, Hay DF. Developmental approaches to aggression and conduct problems. In M. Rutter & D. F. Hay (Eds.), Development through life: A handbook for clinicians. Oxford, England: Blackwell. 1993,488-516
    33. Mitchell P., Acquiring a Conception of Mind, A Review of Psychological Research and Theory, Hove , East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press, Erlbaum, 1996
    34. Nagin D, Tremblay R. Parental and early childhood predictors of persistent physical aggression in boys from kindergarten to high school. Arch Gen Psychiatray, 2001, 58: 389-394
    35. Nijman, H. Muris, P. Merckelbach, H. et al. the staff observation aggression scale-revised(SOAR-R). Aggressive behavior. 1999, 25:197-209
    36. O'Connor D., Archer, J. Wu, F. Measuring Aggressive: Self-reports, partner reports, and responses to provoking scenarios. Aggressive behavior. 2001, 27:79-101
    37. Osterman, K. Bjorkqvist, K. Lagerspetz, K. et al. Cross-cultural evidence of femal indirect aggression. Aggressive behavior. 1998,24:1-8
    38. Parke, R.D., Slaby, R.G.; The Development of Aggression, Handbook of Child Psychology, Fourth Edition, 1983; 547-620
    39. Pepler, D.J., Craig, W. M. A peek behind the fence: Naturalistic observations of aggressive children with remote audiovisual recording. Developmental Psychology, 1995 31: 548-553
    40. Perner, J., Leekman, S., Wimmer, H. Three-year-olds' difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1987,5:125-137
    41. Premack D. & Woodruff G. 'Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1978, 1:515-526
    42. Rivers, I, & Smith, PK: Types of Bullying Behavior and Their Correlates. Aggressive Behavior, 1994; 20:359-368
    43. Schwartz D, Proctor L. Community violence exposure as a predictor of aggression and peer victimization. 1999
    44. Smith, P.K., Boulton, M.J. rough-and tumble play, aggression and dominance: perception and behavior in children's encounters. Human Development, 1990, 33: 271-282
    
    
    45. Sutton, J., Smith, P.K., & Swettenham, J.. Bullying and theory of mind: A critique of the social skills deficit view of anti social behavior. Social development, 1999,8(1):117-127
    46. Tremblay R E, Loeber R, Gagnon C. Charlebois, P., Larivee, S., & LeBlanc, M., Disruptive boys with stable and unstable high fighting behavior patterns during junior elementary school. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 1991, 19:285-300
    47. Tremblay, R. E. The development of aggressive behavior during childhood: What have we learned in the past century, international journal of behavioral development, 2000,6:129-141
    48. Vuchinick S, Bank L, Patterson G. Parenting, peers, and the stability of antisocial behavior in preadolescent boys. Developmental psychology. 1992, 28(3):510-521
    49. Wellman HM, Cross D, Watson JK. A meta-analysis of theory of mind development: The truth about false belief. Poster presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Albuquerque, NM. 1999
    50. Whitney I, Smith PK. A survey of the nature and extent of bullying in junior/middle and secondary schools. Educational Research, 1993, 35:3-25
    51. Wimmer H, Perner J. Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 1983,13: 103-128
    52.董奇.心理与教育研究方法.广东教育出版社.1992
    53.劳伦茨.《攻击与人性》.作家出版社.1987
    54.王益文,张文新.3~6岁儿童“心理理论”的发展.心理发展与教育.2002,18(1):11-15
    55.张文新,张福建.学前儿童在园攻击性行为的观察研究.心理发展与教育,1996,12(4):18-22
    56.张文新.儿童社会性发展.北京师范大学出版社.1999

© 2004-2018 中国地质图书馆版权所有 京ICP备05064691号 京公网安备11010802017129号

地址:北京市海淀区学院路29号 邮编:100083

电话:办公室:(+86 10)66554848;文献借阅、咨询服务、科技查新:66554700